Skip to main content
  • Systematic Review
  • Open access
  • Published:

A Proposed Framework to Describe Movement Variability within Sporting Tasks: A Scoping Review

Abstract

Movement variability is defined as the normal variations in motor performance across multiple repetitions of a task. However, the term “movement variability” can mean different things depending on context, and when used by itself does not capture the specifics of what has been investigated. Within sport, complex movements are performed repeatedly under a variety of different constraints (e.g. different situations, presence of defenders, time pressure). Movement variability has implications for sport performance and injury risk management. Given the importance of movement variability, it is important to understand the terms used to measure and describe it. This broad term of “movement variability” does not specify the different types of movement variability that are currently being assessed in the sporting literature. We conducted a scoping review (1) to assess the current terms and definitions used to describe movement variability within sporting tasks and (2) to utilise the results of the review for a proposed framework that distinguishes and defines the different types of movement variability within sporting tasks. To be considered eligible, sources must have assessed a sporting movement or skill and had at least one quantifiable measure of movement variability. A total of 43 peer-reviewed journal article sources were included in the scoping review. A total of 280 terms relating to movement variability terminology were extracted using a data-charting form jointly developed by two reviewers. One source out of 43 (2%) supplied definitions for all types of movement variability discussed. Moreover, 169 of 280 terms (60%) were undefined in the source material. Our proposed theoretical framework explains three types of movement variability: strategic, execution, and outcome. Strategic variability describes the different approaches or methods of movement used to complete a task. Execution variability describes the intentional and unintentional adjustments of the body between repetitions within the same strategy. Outcome variability describes the differences in the result or product of a movement. These types emerged from broader frameworks in motor control and were adapted to fit the movement variability needs in sports literature. By providing specific terms with explicit definitions, our proposed framework can ensure like-to-like comparisons of previous terms used in the literature. The practical goal of this framework is to aid athletes, coaches, and support staff to gain a better understanding of how the different types of movement variability within sporting tasks contribute to performance. The framework may allow training methods to be tailored to optimise the specific aspects of movement variability that contribute to success. This review was retrospectively registered using the Open Science Framework (OSF) Registries (https://osf.io/q73fd).

Key Points

  1. 1.

    “Movement variability” is a broad term which is often used in the sport literature without a specific definition of the type of movement variability under investigation.

  2. 2.

    A theoretical framework has been proposed to distinguish three types of movement variability: Strategic variability describes the different approaches or methods of movement used to complete a task. Execution variability describes the intentional and unintentional adjustments of the body between repetitions within the same strategy. Outcome variability describes the differences in the result or product of a movement.

  3. 3.

    The terms provided by the proposed framework can increase the specificity of the type of movement variability investigated which can enhance literature comparisons and aid in practical applications.

Introduction

Human movement is variable. Even highly skilled individuals who consistently achieve the same outcome show movement variability within a goal-oriented action or task [1, 2]. The broad term “movement variability” has been defined as “the normal variations that occur in motor performance across multiple repetitions of a task” [3]. This definition and subsequent research on the topic have led to a change in the view of variability from being noise or an error to be minimised, into an advantageous attribute [4, 5]. Advantages of movement variability include enhanced task performance [4, 6,7,8,9,10,11,12] and the spreading of physiological strain [5, 13,14,15,16,17,18,19], both of which are discussed in the following sections. This article focuses specifically on within-individual (also referred to as intra-individual or within-subject) movement variability within sporting tasks. This is to say movement variability that is present when a single person performs the same goal-based task across multiple repetitions [20]. Moreover, given that sport research and performance focus primarily on measuring mechanical variables (e.g. kinematics and kinetics) and their results, this review is focused on mechanical variables of movement and movement variability.

Movement Variability and Performance

Most sporting tasks seek stability and consistency in achieving the desired result (or “goal”) across multiple repetitions [7, 8, 21,22,23]. Tasks do not specify the movements to achieve this result, thus movement variability allows this goal to be consistently achieved despite potential constraints (e.g. situation, defenders, pressure) [4, 5, 7, 8, 12, 21,22,23,24,25,26]. Research has shown that elite performers who achieve stable task results are better at changing their movements to meet the different environments or physiological conditions (e.g. fatigue) that can occur within sporting tasks [4, 11, 12, 23, 24]. For example, basketball athletes have been shown to alter their movements within a jumping task to maintain the same jump height even under the constraint of fatigue [25]. Moreover, elite baseball players displayed increased timing variability on their batting swing resulting in more accurate and frequent hits when compared to novice batters [11]. Within baseball pitchers, it has been proposed that reduced movement variability produces greater consistency of ball location, making pitches more predictable and, as a result, easier to hit [27]. Thus, there is a keen interest in high-performance sporting environments to measure and use movement variability to enhance performance.

Movement Variability and Injury

Movement variability has applications beyond performance such as its effect on physiological strain and stress. James and colleagues [19, 20, 28] proposed the variability-overuse injury hypothesis with variability existing to redistribute stress to other tissues and avoid exceeding physiological capacity. This hypothesis has since been expanded upon by other researchers (e.g. [17, 29]), building on the well-established mechanism of overuse injuries [20, 30,31,32]. The variability-overuse injury hypothesis also suggests movement variability may be a method to mitigate these injuries [32]. This is particularly relevant for sporting performance where high tissue forces and repetitive actions are commonplace. Variability is a way to redistribute the repeated high forces to different tissues over time [17, 20]. An example of this is a volleyball athlete repeatedly landing from jumps. Low variability in this scenario may result in repeated application of strain to the same tissue, increasing the risk of subsequent tissue breakdown or injury due to overuse [20].

Acute injuries may also occur when excessive movement variability is present [33, 34]. Excessive movement variability may result in risky behaviours or more unstable actions being attempted [33, 34]. Excessively high variability is associated with increased exposure to unexpected or erratic position changes as well as novel and unfamiliar movements [33, 34]. It is theorised that exceeding a certain high limit of variability provides an unfamiliar stimulus and the ability to effectively control movements is exceeded [18]. For example, a sprinter with high step width variability may be more likely to fall or negatively compensate trying to maintain balance [5, 35]. Research has shown that a group of cross-country skiers performing an indoor skiing task began displaying high movement variability with fatigue [33]. This suggests fatigued performers may produce highly irregular and unpredictable movements [33, 34]. These factors are all potential precursors to an acute injury scenario [33, 34]. Together this information suggests a theoretical goldilocks zone of movement variability may exist regarding injuries [5, 14, 34]. This optimal zone would exist between too little variability (overuse injury risk) and too much variability (acute injury risk) and has been proposed to follow an inverted-U shape. [5, 14, 34].

Types and Analysis of Movement Variability

There are numerous ways to assess and analyse movement variability for any given task [5, 21, 36,37,38,39,40]. This is because variability is pervasive throughout the multiple levels of movement organisation and can occur in many unique ways [4, 5, 15, 23, 34, 41,42,43,44,45]. For example, within the task of basketball shooting, over multiple attempts it is possible to assess variability in: the choice of shot type, the forces applied during each shot release, or the resultant accuracy of each shot, among many more options. These can all be considered movement variability; however, each of these examples represents a different type of movement variability [4, 20, 44].

Issues also exist when analysing movement variability, as numerous linear and nonlinear statistical methods can be applied (e.g. [5, 12, 15, 23, 36, 38, 39, 41, 43, 45,46,47,48]). Another factor to consider is that variability can be quantified either as a measure of magnitude or a measure of structure [5, 34, 37, 40]. Some quantification techniques, such as nonlinear statistical methods, are sensitive to and consider data structure, whilst other methods do not [3, 36]. Figure 1 provides a visual example of how data can exist with distinctly different data structures despite the same magnitude of variability. Each analysis method has specific considerations as to how it represents movement variability [36,37,38,39,40]. How variability is quantified and what level of movement organisation is measured often reveal what type of movement variability is being investigated [5, 21, 34, 37, 40, 45]. Thus, what is measured and how it is measured are crucial to understand the specifics of the movement variability investigation. These distinctions are necessary because the often-used term “movement variability” is a broad term that does not capture the specifics of what has been investigated.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Reproduced from Komar et al. [12], with permission

Example of a time series data with the same magnitude of variability (mean = 0, standard deviation = 1) but different structures.

Movement Variability Terminology

The term “movement variability” can mean different things depending on the context [4, 5, 9, 12, 16, 17, 42, 43, 49]. Thus, the specific type of movement variability investigated needs to be defined within each context [21]. Failure to define terms may result in similar terms being used interchangeably but with different meanings. The lack of clarity causes difficulties in interpretation and comparison for both readers and researchers. Any misinterpretations from the research could negatively impact practitioners as Preatoni et al. [37] state “the quantification, synthesis, and meaning of movement variability are very important in depicting the athlete’s status and can influence the practical decisions made in sport”.

The following example of anterior cruciate ligament injuries provides evidence of different uses of the same term. The research provided evidence that there is a heightened risk of contralateral anterior cruciate ligament injury post-anterior cruciate ligament reconstruction [50,51,52]. Some research has taken a systems-based approach to the condition explaining there is low movement variability due to the adoption of changed (i.e. compensatory) movements during the rehabilitation process. Low movement variability in this situation is explained by the compensatory movements, which have turned into a rigid, learned behaviour and in turn a neuromuscular system issue [53, 54]. On the other hand, researchers have also taken a tissue capacity approach to explaining the problem. They have argued there is low movement variability due to a lack of tissue capability in the injured limb, which results in increased reliance on the other limb to accommodate [54,55,56]. These are both referred to as “movement variability” but represent different levels of movement organisation and provide different information [44]. Distinguishing the type of movement variability within each context is important as practical applications need to be tailored to target specific adaptations [5, 37].

Issues may also occur with the term “movement variability” when comparing research findings. There is the potential for the literature to be compared based on this term even though the comparisons may not be appropriate. Research from Miller [57, 58] and Robins [59, 60] on basketball shooting found both low and high movement variability can result in successful task performance. Each of these studies have used similar tasks and the same term “movement variability”, but they have investigated fundamentally different aspects of movement variability. This can present an issue when studies are compared as the different interpretations of the same term do not provide like-to-like comparisons (e.g. [4, 8]). These examples show how the term “movement variability” needs to be explicitly defined within each specific context to ensure proper interpretation [4, 5, 8, 21, 36,37,38,39,40]. Given the range of interpretations of this term within the sport literature, a review is needed to determine if and how it is being defined in the current sport literature.

Scoping Review

Outline

The objectives of this scoping review were twofold: (1) to assess the current terms and definitions used to describe movement variability within sporting tasks and (2) to utilise the results of the review for a proposed framework that distinguishes and defines the different types of movement variability within sporting tasks. Use of this framework may assist in interpreting, contrasting, and applying current research through the synthesis of terms and definitions.

Methods

We chose a systematic scoping review to map the current state of terms used in the literature. Due to the large and complex nature of the topic, a scoping review was chosen as it avoids appraising study designs and instead summarises the key concepts [61, 62]. The search strategy was framed by the PRISMA-ScR (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses extension for Scoping Reviews) checklist [63]. This review was registered on 18 November 2020 using the Open Science Framework (OSF) Registries (https://osf.io/q73fd).

The scoping review was conducted on English language, peer-reviewed, and published research journal articles using three searches:

  1. 1.

    “movement variability” AND type AND sport

  2. 2.

    “movement variability” AND term* AND sport

  3. 3.

    “movement variability” AND defin* AND sport

These search terms were selected to provide a representative sample of movement variability in the sporting literature. As discussed in the introduction, movement variability is often referred to as different types [38, 44]. We wished to capture these types, the terms used, and any specific definitions which may be present.

All searches were conducted on 1 January 2022. All years were considered, and searches applied equivalent subjects and related words. A total of five databases were searched; CINAHL Complete, Education Source, MEDLINE Complete, SPORTDiscus, and PubMed. These databases were identified as being relevant to capture multidisciplinary views to movement variability within sporting tasks including what definitions and terms are currently used in the literature. Sources were screened to ensure an appropriate full-text article was available. Sources were eligible if they met the two following eligibility criteria:

  1. 1.

    The source contains at least one quantifiable measure of movement variability

  2. 2.

    The source assesses a sporting movement or skill

These eligibility criteria ensured the scoping review stayed relevant to sporting tasks, and each measure could be objectively identified.

Following screening the remaining eligible information sources were read, and the following data items were extracted:

  • The task performed

  • The format of collected data (e.g. kinematics, kinetics, outcome, etc.)

  • Any explicitly defined terms relating to variability

  • Any implicitly defined terms relating to variability

  • Any undefined terms relating to variability

Terms were considered explicitly defined if they were associated with a clear definition in the text of the source. Terms were considered implicitly defined by meeting two criteria; no explicit definition was provided; however, an equation or rationale that explained how variability was determined was provided in the source. A data-charting form was jointly developed and used by two reviewers to determine which items to extract. The two reviewers independently charted the data, discussed the results, and continuously updated the data-charting form in an iterative process. Data were critically appraised using descriptive statistics in R (version 3.6.0) [64] to assess the number of defined and undefined terms.

Results

A total of 158 sources were identified, of which 49 duplicates were removed resulting in 109 sources to be screened. Through initial screening, eight sources were removed as no full text was available (e.g. conference abstract with no relevant or subsequent paper from authors). All remaining 101 full-text sources were screened for eligibility resulting in the removal of an additional 58 sources. This resulted in 43 sources which were included in the review. A PRISMA diagram of this process is shown in Fig. 2, and a table summarising the included sources is presented in Table 1.

Fig. 2
figure 2

source inclusion for review. Adapted from: Moher et al. [65]

PRISMA diagram showing

Table 1 Defined and undefined terms in movement variability literature focused on sporting skills

One source out of 43 (2%) provided definitions for all terms relating to movement variability discussed within the source context. Of the 43 sources investigating sporting tasks, there were 280 terms relating to movement variability. Of these 280 terms, 111 (40%) were defined, while 169 (60%) terms were undefined. From the 111 terms that were defined, 74 (67%) were explicitly defined in the source, while 37 (33%) were implicitly defined. Kinematics were the primary format of data collection, with 34 out of 43 sources (81%) including some measure of kinematics. Outcome results were also investigated in 10 sources (23%) and kinetics in nine sources (21%).

Discussion

The scoping review provides evidence that a large range of terms are used to describe movement variability within sporting tasks. However, few definitions are provided in relation to each specific context. These results are consistent with prior literature which has concluded that various terms and lack of definitions may be contributing to difficulties in interpretation [4, 5, 8, 17, 36]. The difficulty in interpretation is further supported by Stergiou and Decker [5] who state “much of the controversy that exists in the literature with respect to human movement variability stems from the methodology used”. Similar concerns have been raised by Caballero et al. [36] who claim “the use of so many variables to assess the motor variability have caused problems with the lack of specificity about what variability is”. Without clear definition of these terms in context, it is possible that research may compare the same term despite representing contrasting types of movement variability [4, 8, 17, 57,58,59,60].

We found that the same terms were used across multiple sources but with diverse meanings. The term “coordination variability” is an example of how the same term can be used in two different contexts, be supported by different data analysis approaches, and represent different types of movement variability. “Coordination variability” is a common term both defined and undefined across sources (n = 7). For example, in Komar et al. [82], “coordination variability” is used to distinguish the different clusters of technical solutions shown to perform the task. Based on their technique and performance variables, participants were assigned to a cluster indicating the solution they used (Fig. 3a). In comparison, Irwin et al. [81] used the same term to describe the point-by-point changes in a time series of joint angles over repeated trials of a skill (Fig. 3b). Despite the same term being used, the type of movement variability investigated is different between the studies. Like “movement variability”, the term “coordination variability” used in isolation does not capture the specifics of what has been investigated. The use of the same term to explain different types of movement variability may contribute to a lack of consensus and generalisability within the literature [4, 5, 8,9,10, 12, 17, 22, 23, 34, 36, 37, 40, 44, 46, 47, 79, 82, 102, 105,106,107,108,109,110]. Similar issues can arise when placing many terms in front of the word variability. For example, the term “joint variability” is used in multiple sources without definition. Without definition or adequate context, this term may be interpreted as the changes in the position of the joint as it ends a task, the joint used to perform the task, or how the position of the joint is changing during the task.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Adapted from Komar et al. [82], with permission. b Continuous relative profiles and associated SD of the Trunk-Thigh (hips), Thigh-Shank (knees), and Arm-Trunk (shoulders) plotted simultaneously. Notes: Axis: x = shoulder, z = knees, y = hips. Angular position of the gymnasts denoted via colour coding. Adapted from Irwin et al. [81], with permission

Comparison of the use of the term “coordination variability” across research. a Correspondence between clusters’ participants in highly constrained environment (dark grey) and weakly constrained environment (grey).

Results of the scoping review suggest that researchers need to be more specific and explicit in defining what type of movement variability they are investigating. Currently, the state of the literature requires investigation of individual sources to understand how each term was interpreted [21]. The non-specific and non-explicit use of terms is not a new problem, as Newell [111] discussed similar issues within motor control research and application. Newell [111] showed that multiple terms (“coordination”, “control”, and “skill”) were being used interchangeably with different interpretations. This led to the development of a framework that specified each term and explicitly defined them in order to distinguish investigation areas [111]. A similar framework is needed within the movement variability in sport literature; however, sport settings also present specific challenges which need to be considered.

Sporting tasks need to be assessed within ecologically valid domains, such as during competition [22, 24, 112]. Removing sporting tasks from their target domain has been shown to cause individuals to produce different movements despite the same task [24, 113,114,115,116,117]. Understanding how these task solutions change (or do not change) within the target setting and under different conditions is important for skilled performance and injury risk [4, 5, 8, 14, 17, 20, 22, 28, 33, 34, 53]. It is important for practitioners to understand these changes to inform training methods to enhance performance and decrease injury risk [5, 14, 17, 29, 34, 118,119,120,121,122]. Thus, a framework to specify and explicitly define the types of movement variability should consider these challenges to ensure it applies to sporting settings.

Theoretical Framework for Describing Movement Variability

Outline

Movement variability is an over-arching complex measure comprised of several different types of variability [4, 7, 9, 14, 18, 21, 23, 38, 39, 41, 43, 45]. The scoping review covered in “Scoping Review” section shows that many terms are used in the literature to describe the distinct types of movement variability; however, they are not well defined. Our framework provides specific terms with explicit definitions to describe the different types of movement variability within sporting tasks. As defined by Crick and Koch [123], “a framework is not a detailed hypothesis or set of hypotheses; rather, it is a suggested point of view for an attack on a scientific problem, often suggesting testable hypotheses”. Rycroft-Malone and Bucknall [124] elaborate on this by stating, “their purpose is in providing a frame of reference, for organising thinking, as a guide for what to focus on, and for interpretation”.

Our proposed framework distinguishes three types of movement variability found in the literature: strategic variability, execution variability, and outcome variability. Strategic variability describes the different approaches or methods of movement used to complete a task. Execution variability describes the intentional and unintentional adjustments of the body between repetitions, within the same strategy. Finally, outcome variability describes the differences in the result or product of a movement. A visual representation of this framework is presented in Fig. 4, with definitions of each type of variability explained in the following sections. Moving left to right the framework follows the current understanding of movement action to produce a result. Applying the framework can be summarised as follows: to solve a movement problem an athlete will select a strategy from a pool of appropriate strategies that suit the athlete’s constraints, environment, and task. The strategy is then executed by moving the body in a certain manner to produce a resultant outcome. The terminology in the proposed framework should provide a more specific description of the type of movement variability investigated, and it can also help reduce different interpretations of the same term (see “Types and Analysis of Movement Variability” and “Movement Variability Terminology” section). “A Framework to Describe Movement Variability within Sporting Tasks” section outlines the proposed theoretical framework, and “Application of the Framework: Practical Examples” section provides practical examples to show how the framework can be applied in different settings.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Theoretical framework for describing movement variability with a basketball shot example

A Framework to Describe Movement Variability within Sporting Tasks

Strategic Variability

Strategic variability describes the different approaches or methods of movement used to complete a task. As defined by Bates [125] “a strategy is a selected musculoskeletal solution for the performance of a motor task. Strategy selection can be voluntary or involuntary”. The strategies available to someone during a task are often based on the relevant environmental constraints, and the individual ability to perform the action needed [23, 125,126,127,128,129]. Strategies exist as task-dependent categorical classifications, and to be considered different, strategies must be qualitatively or quantitatively distinct from one another [130]. The quantity of strategic variability of an individual can then be measured via the number of strategies used to perform the task across repetitions. For example, when observing the outcome of throwing a ball to hit a target, the individual may perform some throws overhand and some throws sidearm; this is an example of strategic variability as the strategy to perform the task has changed across repetitions. Similarly, in soccer when kicking to a teammate, an athlete may kick a lob pass, or a direct pass. These different kick types are different strategies that when performed for the same task show strategic variability. These examples of strategic variability are all qualitatively distinct, but strategies can also be quantitatively distinct from one another.

The determination of strategies is dependent on the research question or application, and different strategies may not always be visually distinct. Other measures and methods may be used to determine the strategies’ categorical classification [131,132,133,134,135,136,137]. Clear statements or definitions by the researcher/practitioner of how the strategies are categorised can reduce grey areas between strategies, particularly when strategies may not be as distinctive. An example of this is when multiple strategies reflect two locations on a continuum. Corcos et al. [130] explain this concept using a baseball pitching example, “different variations are likely to be found between a fast ball and a curve ball and these two patterns of variation are probably more understandable as qualitatively different than if lumped together as a function of pitch speed…The two strategies are different in the same sense that the two kinds of pitches are different, and the fact that there are probably movements and pitches that fall between the two extremes does not invalidate the useful notions implicit in creating conceptual categories in the first place”. In sporting literature, strategies within a task have both been defined a priori (before the movement) with pre-determined criteria [15, 18] or a posteriori (after the movement) using data analysis methods to group quantitatively similar performances such as clustering or principal components analysis [131,132,133,134,135,136,137]. For example, the term “coordination variability” by Komar et al. [82] discussed in “Discussion” section used a cluster analysis technique on discrete metrics to retrospectively determine performers’ strategies (Fig. 3a). This is an examination of strategic variability according to the proposed framework as retrospectively the movements are quantitatively distinct, despite visually being similar.

Execution Variability

Execution variability describes the intentional and unintentional adjustments of the body between repetitions within the same strategy. This relates to the small variations that occur even when trying to complete the exact same movement identically [1, 4, 5, 8]. Bernstein [1] explained this phenomenon as “repetition without repetition”, (i.e. repeating the same task without following the exact same formulaic execution). Execution variability is the most common type of movement variability that is investigated in the current literature. An example of execution variability would be the changes in knee and hip angle coordination, which even when running on a fixed speed treadmill show changes over strides [138, 139]. Unlike strategies which must end in categorical classifications, execution variability can be quantified through many different measurement and analysis techniques (e.g. [4, 5, 9, 12, 16, 17, 21, 42, 43, 49]). The data collected and the analysis method selected to assess execution variability should be specific to the research or application intended. For example, continuous measures such as force–time curves, and discrete measures such as peak force produced by a joint can both be used to measure execution variability. These can be analysed using both linear and nonlinear approaches; for a summary of analysis techniques, refer to the following references [5, 21, 36,37,38,39].

Execution variability needs to be quantified within a single strategy. Quantifying execution variability across multiple strategies can produce inflated variability measures. For example, comparing the changes in wrist angle over multiple forehand shots in tennis demonstrates a measure of execution variability. However, combining the analysis of a forehand and a backhand together would provide large amounts of variability due to the different strategies used across repetitions. Comparisons such as this are difficult to interpret as the execution variability is confounded by two strategies. To demonstrate this, Fig. 5a shows six time-normalised vertical ground reaction forces from a vertical jump task (where the individual jumps as high as possible to grab a ball) performed with two distinct movement strategies. Jumps one to three are performed with a countermovement, and jumps four to six are performed with no countermovement. These different movement strategies have qualitatively and quantitatively different ground reaction force traces. Figure 5b provides point by point execution variability (represented by mean and standard deviation) where jumps from both strategies are analysed together. This produces large variability (measured by standard deviation) that provides limited insight and is due mainly to combining different strategies into the same analysis. In contrast, Fig. 5c, d shows the jumps separated by movement strategy prior to assessing execution variability. This example highlights why execution variability should be quantified across repetitions within the same strategy, as comparisons across multiple strategies can provide inflated measures that are difficult to interpret.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Examples of strategic and execution variability using vertical ground reaction force traces from a vertical jumping task. All forces are normalised to percentage time and expressed relative to body weight (N/BW = Newtons per body weight). (Unpublished data). a Six vertical ground reaction force traces from two different movement strategies. Jumps one to three performed with a countermovement and jumps four to six performed with no countermovement. b Point-by-point mean and standard deviation of all jumps showing execution variability when jumps are not separated by strategy. c Point-by-point mean and standard deviation showing execution variability when jumps using a countermovement strategy are separated for analysis. d Point-by-point mean and standard deviation showing execution variability when jumps using a non-countermovement strategy are separated for analysis

Outcome Variability

Outcome variability describes the differences in the result or products of movement. An “outcome” is the term used to explain to what was achieved via the movement [1, 23, 37]. Each measure of an outcome needs to relate directly to the task goal but is dependent on the question of interest. What determines the goal of the task, and what outcome measure best exemplifies this? For example, a made basket or missed basket when shooting basketball free throws are examples of outcome measures. Another example of an outcome measure could be the take-off velocity of a golf ball during a tee-off when trying to maximise distance.

Like execution variability, outcome variability can be quantified through many different measurement and analysis techniques [4, 23, 27, 43,44,45]. The type of data collected and how they are analysed should be specific to the research or application intended. For example, approaches such as success rate (as a percentage) and standard deviation may be used to represent outcome variability in the free throw and golf-ball take-off velocity examples, respectively. Beyond the discrete measures mentioned above the resultant continuous trajectory of an object (e.g. a javelin throw flight path) over repeated executions of a movement may also be used to analyse outcome variability. For a summary of analysis techniques, refer to the following references [5, 21, 36,37,38,39].

Application of the Framework: Practical Examples

Basketball Shooting (Open Discrete Task)

The following explains how this framework can be applied to scoring within a basketball game. This situation is representative of an open discrete task according to the definition by Magill [140]. During the game when shooting, a consistent outcome is desired—the ball going through the hoop and registering a goal. The athlete with ball in hand can choose from a pool of finite strategies that they believe will allow them to achieve this goal. These strategies could include a jump shot, lay-up, floater shot, step-back, etc. These strategies are dictated by the performer’s knowledge, skill, and current environmental demands (e.g. defender presence). It is then up to the athlete to select one of the strategies that best fits the current situation to be executed. Consider a scenario where in 10 attempts with the exact same defensive presence and shot location the athlete attempts five jump shots and five floater shots all of which are successful.

In this specific example, strategic variability may be the number of options the performer has available to them prior to the shot. Currently, this is the hardest area to quantify and relies on categorical identification of different shot types shown in similar situations. In the above situation, two strategic options are used (jump shot and floater shot). These are identified a priori and categorised as being qualitatively distinct from one another. Execution variability is a measure of the intentional and unintentional adjustments of the body between repetitions, within the same strategy (e.g. within all floater shot attempts or within all jump shot attempts). This may include aspects such as wrist angles, knee angles, and vertical ground reaction forces and is dependent on the research question. Finally, the ball going in for a basket or missing may be indicative of outcome variability. For this scenario, a successful outcome (scoring a goal) with low outcome variability is ideal and desired.

Landing (Closed Discrete Task)

In this example, a closed discrete task [140] is chosen whereby an athlete is landing from a jump 20 times. Assume kinetics and kinematics are assessed (such as via 3D motion capture and a force platform), and the outcome of interest is minimising peak ground reaction forces. Applying the framework, outcome variability may be determined by the variability of peak ground reaction force magnitudes. If the researcher or practitioner is interested in determining strategic variability, then they would declare how strategies would be determined. In this example, the criterion for determination of strategy is declared a priori by the joint with the greatest amount of energy absorption. This describes a scenario where the execution variables are used to determine and categorise strategies. This has been done in previous studies (see [15, 18, 141,142,143]). In the present example, two quantitatively distinct strategies are categorised knee-dominant landing or hip-dominant landing. Lastly, the variability in the vertical ground reaction force curve within each strategy may be used to assess the execution variability. Figure 6 provides a visual depiction of each type of movement variability mentioned in this example.

Fig. 6
figure 6

Examples of the three types of movement variability and how they are assessed in a landing task. Strategies are determined by the joint with the greatest energy absorption, with two strategies categorised (knee-dominant landing and hip-dominant landing). Execution variability is represented by the continuous point-by-point standard deviation (SD) of time-normalised vertical ground reaction force traces for all landings within the same strategy. Outcome variability is represented by the SD of the outcome measure (peak force) across all strategies and executions. All forces are normalised to percentage time and expressed relative to body weight (N/BW = Newtons per body weight). (Unpublished data)

Within the above practical example, the joint with the most energy absorbed when landing was used to define a strategy. Thus, multiple landings with a knee-dominant landing strategy can have different amounts of energy absorbed and force applied so long as most of the energy absorbed from those landings is at the knee joint. The changes in ground reaction force (e.g. landing one = 3.5 Newtons per body weight, landing three = 4 Newtons per body weight, etc.) can indicate execution variability within the same strategy.

As stated in “Strategic Variability” section, another method to determine quantitatively different strategies is using analysis techniques such as clustering or principal component analysis [131,132,133,134,135,136,137]. Applying this approach to the landing waveform data discussed above revealed two distinct clusters in line with current research [143, 144]. These reflected the same categories as the a priori declaration but present another way to determine quantitative distinction and determination of strategies.

Running (Continuous Task)

Continuous or cyclical tasks such as walking, running, cycling, and rowing pose unique situations to this framework. Often during these tasks, the execution is itself the outcome, and as such the measure of these types of variability blend [140]. Moreover, depending on the research question or application there may be no strategic variability within a continuous task. However, as explained in “Strategic Variability” section, strategies may also be visually or qualitatively similar despite showing quantitative distinctions [131,132,133,134,135,136,137].

Consider the example of running where kinetics and kinematics are collected over 50 stride cycles on a treadmill. There is no set outcome/specific goal to the movement outside of maintaining running velocity and executing the movement itself. If strategic variability is of interest to the researcher or practitioner, then in this example, different strategies may be identified retroactively through the frequency of forefoot, midfoot, or heel-strike ground contact patterns. Each of these types of foot strikes can be determined quantitatively via their distinct vertical ground reaction force traces [145,146,147,148,149]. The execution variability may be identified by assessing the variability of the biomechanics within each of these strategies. For example, this may involve looking at the continuous knee joint angle changes of each step during the forefoot strike strategy.

Discussion of Theoretical Framework

How the Framework Fits with Previous Approaches

Utilising a framework to distinguish the types of movement variability is not new [2, 14, 21, 22, 37, 83, 110, 111, 121, 122]. However, the proposed framework in this paper builds upon several earlier frameworks and considers elements that were not addressed previously. This framework provides an explicit consideration of strategic movement variability which is not considered in previous frameworks. This issue was raised by Newell [111] within his paper by stating “the framework proposed does not address directly the issue of strategy although clearly it is an important element of skilled performance”. This paper shares many similar motivations with the paper by Newell [111] who also developed a framework to create distinction between the three terms of “coordination”, “control”, and “skill”. Like the findings in “Scoping Review” section, Newell stated “the distinction between the terms coordination, control and skill is not apparent. Furthermore, perusal of the many academic texts on motor skill learning and motor control reveals a disparity of perspectives on the meaning and significance of these three concepts to the extent there are virtually as many definitions as sources. This inconsistency exists both within a given level of analysis of action (e.g., behavioural) and in a consideration between levels of analysis, such as behavioural and physiological” [111]. By providing a framework that distinguished the terms “coordination”, “control”, and “skill”, Newell outlined how each term represents a unique level of movement organisation [111].

Related frameworks developed by Saltzman and Kelso [121] and Ranganathan and Newell [110] also identified that there is a need to specify terms and distinguish their different interpretations. Each of these frameworks differentiated the result of the movement from the movement itself [110, 121]. Saltzman and Kelso [121] provided descriptive levels of action which separated the outcome of movement, from the body spatial elements involved in the movement, and how these elements were organised. Ranganathan and Newell [110] used a framework to highlight how variability can occur at the level of the task goal (the requirements of the task) and in execution redundancy (the ability to achieve the same task outcome). These descriptions were applied to a sporting example and showed how multiple solutions to the same task can occur despite achieving the same task outcome [110]. Both approaches parallel research by Scholz et al. [122] who applied the terms “essential” and “non-essential” variables to distinguish the variables that influence the task outcome and the variables that do not [150]. They found that to ensure low outcome variability, certain variables needed to display low variability, but other variables were able to show high variability without influencing outcome variability [122, 150]. Each of these approaches enhanced the understanding of skilled movements as each concluded that understanding and analysis at each part of the respective frameworks provided a different and specific understanding of movement [110, 111, 121, 122, 150].

In movement variability research clearly identifying and defining what is being investigated is of high importance. A lack of specificity of terminology and definitions has been previously noted to cause confusion [4, 5, 8, 9, 12, 17, 22, 34, 36, 40, 44, 46, 47, 79, 105, 106, 108,109,110,111]. As such, the proposed framework shares an analogous approach to earlier frameworks which aimed to enhance clarity and specificity around what is being investigated and how it influences skilled performance [110, 111, 121, 122, 150]. The proposed framework in this paper aims to address this by providing specific terms with explicit definitions which can be used to imply context across settings. Furthermore, the framework introduced in this paper allows for specific application into sporting environments, which can often present unique challenges such as constantly changing task constraints [22]. Being able to assess skilled movements within the intended performance domain is of key interest in the literature [24, 112,113,114,115,116,117].

In applying this framework to the scoping review literature, a breakdown of the specific types of movement variability investigated within each study is shown:

  • Ten studies provided at least one measure of all three types of movement variability [7, 24, 67,68,69,70, 80, 89, 91, 104].

  • Four studies provided at least one measure of outcome and execution variability [84, 85, 92, 101].

  • 13 studies provided at least one measure of strategic and execution variability [12, 66, 72, 73, 76, 77, 82, 83, 93,94,95, 98, 99].

  • 15 studies investigated only execution variability [14, 74, 75, 78, 79, 81, 86,87,88, 90, 96, 97, 100, 102, 103].

  • One study investigated only outcome variability [71].

By applying the framework, terms that were identified as undefined through the scoping review are now grouped based on the type of movement variability investigated. In doing so, this framework provides a method to specify how the terms are being used, which may create opportunities for more like-to-like comparisons of terms within the literature. Furthermore, being able to distinguish between diverse types of movement variability investigated can provide insights into how movement is organised in sport settings [22]. Application of the framework can be used to help specify and distinguish changes within the different types of movement variability. This can be useful information to guide and help practical applications in sport such as improving sporting performance, mitigating injury risk, and maximising rehabilitation results.

Practical Considerations

The practical goal of this framework is to aid athletes, coaches, and support staff to gain a better understanding of how the different types of movement variability within sporting tasks contribute to performance. This will allow training methods to be tailored to optimise the specific aspects of movement variability that contribute to success and minimise the others. Ranganathan et al. [22] have stated that “although there are plenty of examples of elite players changing their movement pattern to improve performance or reduce injury, there is little information available on the process of how this reorganisation occurs”. The proposed framework provides a method to help understand how and where this reorganisation is occurring. Furthermore, the proposed framework also aligns with the approach of Ranganathan et al. [22] who identified that in sport settings, changes in movements fall into either explicit, “strategy-like” behaviours or implicit, “synergy-like” behaviours. The strategy-like and synergy-like behaviours align with the proposed strategic and execution types of movement variability, respectively. Distinguishing these types of movement variability is important as expert performers display consistent outcomes despite multiple means of completing a task, i.e. they display both strategic and execution variability [4, 7, 8, 12, 22, 23, 83]. By distinguishing these types from one another, research may be able to yield greater understanding of how expert performers organise their movements when performing tasks in their specific domains [4, 7, 8, 12, 22, 23, 83].

If an individual displays low strategic or execution variability, this lack of adaptability may be exploited by the opposition [4, 8, 13, 24, 27]. For example, a basketball player may have high success with low outcome variability when they drive to the basket with their right hand; however, being forced to their left hand by defenders they have higher outcome variability and thus less success. Defenders aware of this could then heavily guard the right hand forcing more left-hand drives and thus reduced success. Identifying these deficiencies allows for coaches and support staff to implement training methods to develop these abilities [88, 91, 151, 152]. Barris et al. [7] promoted execution variability within the diving training environment and found not only increases in execution variability but also increases in performance consistency post-intervention. This was achieved despite some initial resistance from coaches [7]. Typically, coaches have viewed a successful, low outcome variability as being related to a rigid technical model (low execution and strategic variability) [1, 4, 5, 7, 8, 12, 22, 23, 42, 43, 83, 110, 152]. The proposed framework in this paper may be used as a tool to help show coaches the different types of movement variability and how it may be advantageous to have high or low execution and strategic variability in different settings [4, 5, 7, 8, 12, 23, 152].

A second practical application of the proposed framework is within sports injury and rehabilitation settings [5, 13, 14, 17, 19, 20, 28, 29, 53, 120]. Providing ways to mitigate the risk of injury and re-injury is a key job role for many practitioners. As discussed in “Movement Variability and Injury” section, there is a theoretical relationship between the magnitude of movement variability and overuse injury [14, 17, 19, 28, 29]. Understanding what type of movement variability is changing, and how, offers insights on if the body is adapting (or not adapting) to demands [5, 33, 34, 132]. The ability to conceptualise what type of movement variability is reduced has implications for athlete management and training interventions. Studies have shown that when injured, under fatigue or under increased task demands the amount of strategic and execution variability is reduced [5, 22, 34, 110, 132]. This suggests stress is being applied repeatedly to the same tissues which may result in injury [17, 20, 30,31,32]. It is also unclear what happens if the task constraints change, and the available strategic and execution variability options are no longer viable. It has been postulated that this may increase the risk of acute injuries [5, 14, 33, 34]. Applying the proposed framework in this paper allows for these, and other theoretical questions to be investigated. For example, it is well established that landing from a jump with limited knee flexion produces a large vertical ground reaction force [143, 153]. This high force is considered a negative by most practitioners, and techniques to reduce these peak vertical ground reaction forces are often taught [142,143,144]. However, it is unknown if the athlete is better off landing with large ground reaction forces if different strategies are used over time (e.g. hip-dominant strategy and knee-dominant strategy)? Or if the athlete is better off performing a strategy that results in lower ground reaction forces but only using one strategy over time? This is to say, is the athlete better off to have high strategic variability and high force, low strategic variability and low force, or another combination? The proposed framework may provide the specific terminology and understanding to help practitioners to explore this question.

Mitigating the risk of injury and re-injury is a key job role for many practitioners, and understanding how the framework can be applied in rehabilitation settings can result in better short-term and long-term outcomes for athletes. Applying this framework allows practitioners to monitor the different types of movement variability to ensure optimal performance and return-to-play criteria. Within rehabilitation settings, it has been suggested that monitoring movement variability may be a more sensitive marker for return to sport than traditional measures [14, 154]. In research by Seay et al. [154] even runners who were considered recovered from low back pain injuries still showed lower execution variability than those who had never been injured. This suggests a potential re-injury mechanism as a smaller section of tissue is being increasingly stressed in line with the stress-overuse injury hypothesis [14, 17, 19, 20, 28,29,30,31,32]. Targeting this specific type of movement variability with training interventions may enhance execution variability and result in better long-term outcomes for these individuals. Similar findings have occurred in individuals with anterior cruciate ligament reconstructions [53, 88, 155,156,157]. Stergiou et al. [157] found that those with reconstructed anterior cruciate ligaments displayed less execution variability in the involved limb when walking compared to the non-involved limb [53, 157, 158]. This movement rigidity is suggested to have implications for osteoarthritis and articular cartilage degeneration as the same articulating surfaces are loaded repeatedly over time [5, 159,160,161]. Thus, applying the framework to understand the types of movement variability and specific interventions for each type may be beneficial to long-term rehabilitation outcomes.

Limitations

This framework is not without limitations, one of which is that human movement is inherently complex [4, 7, 9, 14, 18, 21, 23, 38, 39, 41, 43, 45]. The focus of this review was on the mechanical variables of movement variability; however, there are more types of movement variability (e.g. neural variability), which were not considered that could explain other important aspects of movement and behaviour [41, 162, 163]. Another limitation is that certain tasks may provide grey areas within the framework where distinguishing each type is difficult. The authors have tried to address this issue within the framework design as discussed in “Strategic Variability” section with reference to the work by Corcos et al. [130]. Limitations also exist within the scoping review process as the literature on movement variability is covered in many different fields [5, 21, 34, 41, 43, 108, 162, 163]. Research outside sporting tasks was excluded, which may have potentially missed some applicable studies, terms, and consensus terminology. Furthermore, this review was conducted in a scoping manner due to the large and complex nature of this topic. This limits results as the focus was narrowed to a subset of the available literature. This subset provides evidence that a more precise and comprehensive systematic review is valid, but the feasibility of such a review must also be considered.

Conclusion

The scoping review revealed that “movement variability” is a broad term with many different interpretations within the sporting literature. These terms are often not explicitly defined and therefore do not specify what is being investigated. Thus, a theoretical framework is proposed that distinguishes and defines three distinct types of movement variability within sporting tasks: strategic variability, which describes the different approaches or methods of movement used to complete a task; execution variability, which describes the intentional and unintentional adjustments of the body between repetitions within the same strategy, and outcome variability, which describes the differences in the result or product of a movement. By providing specific terms with explicit definitions, the proposed framework can ensure like-to-like comparisons of previous terms used in the literature. By practically applying this framework, athletes, coaches, and support staff can gain a better understanding of how the distinct types of movement variability within sporting tasks contribute to performance. This allows training methods to be tailored to optimise the specific aspects of movement variability that contribute to success.

Availability of data and materials

The data within this study are secondary data and available through the relevant sources referenced.

References

  1. Bernstein NA. The coordination and regulation of movements. Oxford: Pergamon Press; 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Johnson HW. Skill = speed x accuracy x form x adaptability. Percept Mot Skills. 1961;13(2):163–70. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1961.13.2.163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Stergiou N, Buzzi UH, Kurz MJ, Heidel J. Nonlinear tools in human movement. In: Stergiou N, editor. Innovative analyses of human movement. Champaign: Human Kinetics; 2004. p. 63–92.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Bartlett R, Wheat J, Robins M. Is movement variability important for sports biomechanists? Sports Biomech. 2007;6(2):224–43. https://doi.org/10.1080/14763140701322994.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Stergiou N, Decker LM. Human movement variability, nonlinear dynamics, and pathology: Is there a connection? Hum Mov Sci. 2011;30(5):869–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2011.06.002.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Barris S, Davids K, Farrow D. Representative learning design in springboard diving: is dry-land training representative of a pool dive? Eur J Sport Sci. 2013;13(6):638–45. https://doi.org/10.1080/17461391.2013.770923.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Barris S, Farrow D, Davids K. Increasing functional variability in the preparatory phase of the takeoff improves elite springboard diving performance. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2014;85(1):97–106. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2013.872220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Bartlett R. Movement variability and its implications for sports scientists and practitioners: an overview. Int J Sports Sci Coach. 2008;3(1):113–24. https://doi.org/10.1260/174795408784089397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Busquets A, Marina M, Davids K, Angulo-Barroso R. Differing roles of functional movement variability as experience increases in gymnastics. J Sports Sci Med. 2016;15:268–76.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Cazzola D, Pavei G, Preatoni E. Can coordination variability identify performance factors and skill level in competitive sport? The case of race walking. J Sport Health Sci. 2016;5:35–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jshs.2015.11.005.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Gray R. Changes in movement coordination associated with skill acquisition in baseball batting: freezing/freeing degrees of freedom and functional variability. Front Psychol. 2020;11:1–17. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Komar J, Seifert L, Thouvarecq R. What variability tells us about motor expertise: measurements and perspectives from a complex system approach. Mov Sports Sci. 2015;89:65–77. https://doi.org/10.1051/sm/2015020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Glasgow P, Bleakley CM, Phillips N. Being able to adapt to variable stimuli: the key driver in injury and illness prevention? Br J Sports Med. 2013;47(2):64–5. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2012-091960.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Hamill J, Palmer C, Van Emmerik REA. Coordinative variability and overuse injury. Sports Med Arthrosc Rehabil Ther Technol. 2012;4(1):45–53. https://doi.org/10.1186/1758-2555-4-45.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Nordin AD, Dufek JS. Load accommodation strategies and movement variability in single-leg landing. J Appl Biomech. 2017;33(4):241–7. https://doi.org/10.1123/jab.2016-0097.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Nordin AD, Dufek JS. Lower extremity variability changes with drop-landing height manipulations. Res Sports Med. 2017;25(2):144–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/15438627.2017.1282353.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Nordin AD, Dufek JS. Reviewing the variability-overuse injury hypothesis: Does movement variability relate to landing injuries? Res Q Exerc Sport. 2019;90(2):190–205. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2019.1576837.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Nordin AD, Dufek JS, James CR, Bates BT. Classifying performer strategies in drop landing activities. J Sports Sci. 2017;35(18):1858–63. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2016.1240876.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. James CR, Dufek JS, Bates BT. Effects of injury proneness and task difficulty on joint kinetic variability. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2000;32(11):1833–44. https://doi.org/10.1097/00005768-200011000-00004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. James CR. Considerations of movement variability in biomechanics research. In: Stergiou N, editor. Innovative analyses of human movement. Champaign: Human Kinetics; 2004. p. 29–62.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Newell KM, Corcos DM. Variability and motor control. Champaign: Human Kinetics Publishers; 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Ranganathan R, Lee M-H, Newell KM. Repetition without repetition: challenges in understanding behavioral flexibility in motor skill. Front Psychol. 2020. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02018.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Tucker CB, Anderson R, Kenny IC. Is outcome related to movement variability in golf? Sports Biomech. 2013;12(4):343–54. https://doi.org/10.1080/14763141.2013.784350.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Gorman AD, Maloney MA. Representative design: does the addition of a defender change the execution of a basketball shot? Psychol Sport Exerc. 2016;27:112–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2016.08.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Legg J, Pyne DB, Semple S, Ball N. Variability of jump kinetics related to training load in elite female basketball. Sports. 2017;5(4):85. https://doi.org/10.3390/sports5040085.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Newell KM. Constraints on the development of coordination. In: Wade MG, Whiting HTA, editors. Motor development in children: aspects of coordination and control. Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff; 1986. p. 341–60.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  27. Glanzer JA, Diffendaffer AZ, Slowik JS, Drogosz M, Lo NJ, Fleisig GS. The relationship between variability in baseball pitching kinematics and consistency in pitch location. Sports Biomech. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1080/14763141.2019.1642378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. James CR. Effects of overuse injury proneness and task difficulty on joint kinetic variability during landing. Unpublished doctoral dissertation: University of Oregon, Eugene; 1996.

  29. Hamill J, van Emmerik RE, Heiderscheit BC, Li L. A dynamical systems approach to lower extremity running injuries. Clin Biomech. 1999;14(5):297–308. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0268-0033(98)90092-4.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Nordin M, Frankel VH. Basic biomechanics of the musculoskeletal system. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Williams KR. Biomechanics of distance running. In: Grabiner MD, editor. Current issues in biomechanics. Champaign: Human Kinetics; 1993. p. 3–31.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Zernicke RF, Loitz BJ. Exercise-related adaptations in connective tissue. In: Komi P, editor. Strength and power in sport. Champaign: Human Kinetics; 1992. p. 77–95.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Cignetti F, Schena F, Rouard A. Effects of fatigue on inter-cycle variability in cross-country skiing. J Biomech. 2009;42(10):1452–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2009.04.012.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Stergiou N, Harbourne R, Cavanaugh J. Optimal movement variability: a new theoretical perspective for neurologic physical therapy. J Neurol Phys Ther. 2006;30(3):120–9. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.npt.0000281949.48193.d9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Brach JS, Berlin JE, VanSwearingen JM, Newman AB, Studenski SA. Too much or too little step width variability is associated with a fall history in older persons who walk at or near normal gait speed. J Neuroeng Rehabilit. 2005;2:21. https://doi.org/10.1186/1743-0003-2-21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Caballero C, Barbado D, Moreno FJ. Non-linear tools and methodological concerns measuring human movement variability: an overview. Eur J Hum Mov. 2014;32:61–81. https://doi.org/10.21134/eurjhm.2014.32.319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Preatoni E, Hamill J, Harrison AJ, Hayes K, Van Emmerik RE, Wilson C, et al. Movement variability and skills monitoring in sports. Sports Biomech. 2013;12(2):69–92. https://doi.org/10.1080/14763141.2012.738700.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Stergiou N. Innovative analyses of human movement. Champaign: Human Kinetics; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Stergiou N. Nonlinear analysis for human movement variability. London: CRC Press; 2018.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  40. van Emmerik REA, Ducharme SW, Amado AC, Hamill J. Comparing dynamical systems concepts and techniques for biomechanical analysis. J Sport Health Sci. 2016;5(1):3–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jshs.2016.01.013.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Haar S, Donchin O, Dinstein I. Individual movement variability magnitudes are explained by cortical neural variability. J Neurosci. 2017;37(37):9076–85. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1650-17.2017.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Latash ML. The bliss (not the problem) of motor abundance (not redundancy). Exp Brain Res. 2012;217(1):1–5. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3000-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Latash ML, Scholz JP, Schöner G. Motor control strategies revealed in the structure of motor variability. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2002;30(1):26–31. https://doi.org/10.1097/00003677-200201000-00006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Newell KM, Corcos DM. Issues in variability and motor control. In: Newell KM, Corcos DM, editors. Variability and motor control. Champaign: Human Kinetics Publishers; 1993. p. 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Nimphius S, McGuigan MR, Suchomel TJ, Newton RU. Variability of a “force signature” during windmill softball pitching and relationship between discrete force variables and pitch velocity. Hum Mov Sci. 2016;47:151–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2016.03.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Cardis M, Casadio M, Ranganathan R. High variability impairs motor learning regardless of whether it affects task performance. J Neurophysiol. 2018;119(1):39–48. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00158.2017.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Hamill J, Haddad JM, McDermott WJ. Issues in quantifying variability from a dynamical systems perspective. J Appl Biomech. 2000;16(4):407. https://doi.org/10.1123/jab.16.4.407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Wheat JS, Glazier P. Techniques for measuring coordination and coordination variability. In: Davids K, Bennett SJ, Newell KM, editors. Variability in the movement system: a multi-disciplinary perspective. Champaign: Human Kinetics; 2006. p. 167–81.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Dhawale AK, Smith MA, Ölveczky BP. The role of variability in motor learning. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2017;40:479–98. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-072116-031548.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Kamath GV, Murphy T, Creighton RA, Viradia N, Taft TN, Spang JT. Anterior cruciate ligament injury, return to play, and reinjury in the elite collegiate athlete: analysis of an NCAA division I cohort. Am J Sports Med. 2014;42(7):1638–43. https://doi.org/10.1177/0363546514524164.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Swärd P, Kostogiannis I, Roos H. Risk factors for a contralateral anterior cruciate ligament injury. Knee Surg Sports Traumatol Arthrosc. 2010;18(3):277–91. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00167-009-1026-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Wiggins AJ, Grandhi RK, Schneider DK, Stanfield D, Webster KE, Myer GD. Risk of secondary injury in younger athletes after anterior cruciate ligament reconstruction: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Sports Med. 2016;44(7):1861–76. https://doi.org/10.1177/0363546515621554.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Moraiti C, Stergiou N, Ristanis S, Georgoulis AD. ACL deficiency affects stride-to-stride variability as measured using nonlinear methodology. Knee Surg Sports Traumatol Arthrosc. 2007;15(12):1406–13. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00167-007-0373-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Jordan MJ, Aagaard P, Herzog W. Lower limb asymmetry in mechanical muscle function: a comparison between ski racers with and without ACL reconstruction. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2015;25(3):e301–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.12314.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Turpeinen JT, Freitas TT, Rubio-Arias J, Jordan MJ, Aagaard P. Contractile rate of force development after anterior cruciate ligament reconstruction-a comprehensive review and meta-analysis. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2020;30(9):1572–85. https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.13733.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Courtney C, Rine RM, Kroll P. Central somatosensory changes and altered muscle synergies in subjects with anterior cruciate ligament deficiency. Gait Posture. 2005;22(1):69–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2004.07.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Miller S, editor. The kinematics of inaccuracy in basketball shooting. In: 16 international symposium on biomechanics in sports; 1998.

  58. Miller S, editor. Variability in basketball shooting: practical implications. In: 18 international symposium on biomechanics in sport; 2000.

  59. Robins MT. An investigation into the variability of segmental limb co-ordination of the shooting arm in basketball: UWIC; 2003.

  60. Robins MT, Wheat J, Irwin G, Bartlett R. The effect of shooting distance on movement variability in basketball. J Hum Mov Stud. 2006;50(4):217–38.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Davis K, Drey N, Gould D. What are scoping studies? A review of the nursing literature. Int J Nurs Stud. 2009;46(10):1386–400. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2009.02.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Peters MDJ, Godfrey CM, Khalil H, McInerney P, Parker D, Soares CB. Guidance for conducting systematic scoping reviews. Int J Evid Based Healthc. 2015;13(3):141–6. https://doi.org/10.1097/xeb.0000000000000050.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Tricco AC, Lillie E, Zarin W, O’Brien KK, Colquhoun H, Levac D, et al. PRISMA extension for scoping reviews (PRISMA-ScR): checklist and explanation. Ann Intern Med. 2018;169(7):467–73. https://doi.org/10.7326/M18-0850.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 2009;6(7): e1000097. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Aljohani M, Kipp K. Use of self-organizing maps to study sex- and speed-dependent changes in running biomechanics. Hum Mov Sci. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2020.102649.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Bańkosz Z, Winiarski S. Kinematic parameters of topspin forehand in table tennis and their inter- and intra-individual variability. J Sports Sci Med. 2020;19(1):138–48.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Bobrownicki R, MacPherson AC, Collins D, Sproule J. The acute effects of analogy and explicit instruction on movement and performance. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2019;44:17–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2019.04.016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Chang M, Halaki M, Adams R, Cobley S, Lee K-Y, O’Dwyer N. An exploration of the perception of dance and its relation to biomechanical motion: a systematic review and narrative synthesis. J Dance Med Sci. 2016;20(3):127–36. https://doi.org/10.12678/1089-313X.20.3.127.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Chow JY, Davids K, Button C, Rein R. Dynamics of movement patterning in learning a discrete multiarticular action. Mot Control. 2008;12(3):219–40. https://doi.org/10.1123/mcj.12.3.219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Duarte JE, Reinkensmeyer DJ. Effects of robotically modulating kinematic variability on motor skill learning and motivation. J Neurophysiol. 2015;113(7):2682–91. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00163.2014.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. Fargier P, Massarelli R, Rabahi T, Gemignani A, Fargier E. Fast regulation of vertical squat jump during push-off in skilled jumpers. Front Physiol. 2016;7:289. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2016.00289.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Floria P, Gómez-Landero LA, Harrison AJ. Variability in the application of force during the vertical jump in children and adults. J Appl Biomech. 2014;30(6):679–84. https://doi.org/10.1123/jab.2014-0043.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. García-Pinillos F, Latorre-Román PA, Ramírez-Campillo R, Párraga-Montilla JA, Roche-Seruendo LE. Minimum time required for assessing step variability during running at submaximal velocities. J Biomech. 2018;80:186–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2018.09.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Grassi GP, Santini T, Lovecchio N, Turci M, Ferrario VF, Sforza C. Spatiotemporal consistency of trajectories in gymnastics: a three-dimensional analysis of flic-flac. Int J Sports Med. 2005;26(2):134–8. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2004-817861.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Guignard B, Rouard A, Chollet D, Seifert L. Behavioral dynamics in swimming: the appropriate use of inertial measurement units. Front Psychol. 2017;8:383. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00383.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Harrison AJ, Ryan W, Hayes K. Functional data analysis of joint coordination in the development of vertical jump performance. Sports Biomech. 2007;6(2):199–214. https://doi.org/10.1080/14763140701323042.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Haudum A, Birklbauer J, Müller E. The effect of external perturbations on variability in joint coupling and single joint variability. Hum Mov Sci. 2014;36:246–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2014.02.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Hiley MJ, Zuevsky VV, Yeadon MR. Is skilled technique characterized by high or low variability? An analysis of high bar giant circles. Hum Mov Sci. 2013;32(1):171–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2012.11.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Hodges NJ, Franks IM. Modelling coaching practice: the role of instruction and demonstration. J Sports Sci. 2002;20(10):793–811. https://doi.org/10.1080/026404102320675648.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Irwin G, Kerwin DG, Williams G, Van Emmerik REA, Newell KM, Hamill J. Multidimensional joint coupling: a case study visualisation approach to movement coordination and variability. Sports Biomech. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1080/14763141.2018.1475579.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Komar J, Chow JY, Chollet D, Seifert L. Neurobiological degeneracy: supporting stability, flexibility and pluripotentiality in complex motor skill. Acta Psychol. 2015;154:26–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.11.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Langdown B, Bridge M, Li F-X. Movement variability in the golf swing. Sports Biomech. 2012;11(2):273–87. https://doi.org/10.1080/14763141.2011.650187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Malhotra N, Poolton JM, Wilson MR, Omuro S, Masters RSW. Dimensions of movement specific reinvestment in practice of a golf putting task. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2015;18:1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.11.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  85. Marquardt C. The SAM puttLab: concept and PGA tour data. Int J Sports Sci Coach. 2007;2:101–20. https://doi.org/10.1260/174795407789705479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Maurer LK, Maurer H, Müller H. Analysis of timing variability in human movements by aligning parameter curves in time. Behav Res Methods. 2018;50(5):1841–52. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0952-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Middleton KJ, Wells DJM, Foster DH, Alderson JA. Proximal cueing to reduce elbow extension levels in suspect spin bowlers: a case study. Int J Sports Sci Coach. 2018;13(5):643–8. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747954118792852.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  88. Mohammadi Orangi B, Yaali R, Bahram A, Aghdasi MT, van der Kamp J, Vanrenterghem J, et al. Motor learning methods that induce high practice variability reduce kinematic and kinetic risk factors of non-contact ACL injury. Hum Mov Sci. 2021;78:102805. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2021.102805.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Orth D, McDonic L, Ashbrook C, van der Kamp J. Efficient search under constraints and not working memory resources supports creative action emergence in a convergent motor task. Hum Mov Sci. 2019;67:102505. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2019.102505.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Reeve HK, Hopper LS, Elliott BC, Ackland TR. Lower limb kinematic variability in dancers performing drop landings onto floor surfaces with varied mechanical properties. Hum Mov Sci. 2013;32(4):866–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2013.07.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Santos S, Coutinho D, Gonçalves B, Abade E, Pasquarelli B, Sampaio J. Effects of manipulating ball type on youth footballers’ performance during small-sided games. Int J Sports Sci Coach. 2020;15(2):170–83. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747954120908003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Sayers MGL. The influence of bowling velocity on movement variability in experienced older aged lawn bowlers. J Sci Med Sport. 2019;22(9):1065–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2019.04.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Schaefer A, O’Dwyer N, Ferdinands RED, Edwards S. Consistency of kinematic and kinetic patterns during a prolonged spell of cricket fast bowling: an exploratory laboratory study. J Sports Sci. 2018;36(6):679–90. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2017.1330548.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Seifert L, Wattebled L, Herault R, Poizat G, Adé D, Gal-Petitfaux N, et al. Neurobiological degeneracy and affordance perception support functional intra-individual variability of inter-limb coordination during ice climbing. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(2):e89865-e. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0089865.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  95. Slobounov S, Yukelson D, O’Brien R. Self-efficacy and movement variability of olympic-level springboard divers. J Appl Sport Psychol. 1997;9(2):171–90. https://doi.org/10.1080/10413209708406480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  96. Strongman C, Morrison A. A scoping review of non-linear analysis approaches measuring variability in gait due to lower body injury or dysfunction. Hum Mov Sci. 2020;69:102562. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2019.102562.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Tanaka Y, Sekiya H. The relationships between psychological/physiological changes and behavioral/performance changes of a golf putting task under pressure. Int J Sport Health Sci. 2010;2010(8):83–94. https://doi.org/10.5432/ijshs.20100006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  98. Torres EB. Signatures of movement variability anticipate hand speed according to levels of intent. Behav Brain Funct. 2013;9:10. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-9-10.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  99. Trounson KM, Busch A, French Collier N, Robertson S. Effects of acute wearable resistance loading on overground running lower body kinematics. PLoS ONE. 2020;15(12):e0244361. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0244361.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  100. van Ginneken WF, Poolton JM, Capio CM, van der Kamp J, Choi CSY, Masters RSW. Conscious control is associated with freezing of mechanical degrees of freedom during motor learning. J Mot Behav. 2018;50(4):436–56. https://doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2017.1365045.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Wang W, Qu F, Li S, Wang L. Effects of motor skill level and speed on movement variability during running. J Biomech. 2021;127:110680. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2021.110680.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Wilson C, Simpson SE, Van Emmerik REA, Hamill J. Coordination variability and skill development in expert triple jumpers. Sports Biomech. 2008;7(1):2–9. https://doi.org/10.1080/14763140701682983.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  103. Wren TAL, O’Callahan B, Katzel MJ, Zaslow TL, Edison BR, VandenBerg CD, et al. Movement variability in pre-teen and teenage athletes performing sports related tasks. Gait Posture. 2020;80:228–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2020.06.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Yang JF, Scholz JP. Learning a throwing task is associated with differential changes in the use of motor abundance. Exp Brain Res. 2005;163(2):137–58. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-004-2149-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Knight CA. Neuromotor issues in the learning and control of golf skill. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2004;75(1):9–15. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2004.10609128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Chapman A, Vicenzino B, Blanch P, Hodges P. Do differences in muscle recruitment between novice and elite cyclists reflect different movement patterns or less skilled muscle recruitment? J Sci Med Sport. 2009;12(1):31–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2007.08.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Glazier PS, Davids K. On analysing and interpreting variability in motor output. J Sci Med Sport. 2009;12(4):e2–3. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2009.03.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  108. Lockhart T, Stergiou N. New perspectives in human movement variability. Ann Biomed Eng. 2013;41(8):1593–4. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10439-013-0852-0.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  109. Herzfeld DJ, Shadmehr R. Motor variability is not noise, but grist for the learning mill. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17(2):149–50. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3633.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Ranganathan R, Newell KM. Changing up the routine: intervention-induced variability in motor learning. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2013;41(1):64–70. https://doi.org/10.1097/JES.0b013e318259beb5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  111. Newell KM. Coordination, control and skill. In: Goodman D, Wilberg RB, Franks IM, editors. Advances in psychology. Amsterdam: North-Holland; 1985. p. 295–317.

    Google Scholar 

  112. Wulf G, Shea CH. Principles derived from the study of simple skills do not generalize to complex skill learning. Psychon Bull Rev. 2002;9(2):185–211. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196276.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Pinder RA, Davids K, Renshaw I, Araújo D. Representative learning design and functionality of research and practice in sport. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2011;33(1):146–55. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.33.1.146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  114. Pinder RA, Renshaw I, Davids K. Information-movement coupling in developing cricketers under changing ecological practice constraints. Hum Mov Sci. 2009;28(4):468–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2009.02.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  115. Renshaw I, Oldham ARH, Davids K, Golds T. Changing ecological constraints of practice alters coordination of dynamic interceptive actions. Eur J Sport Sci. 2007;7(3):157–67. https://doi.org/10.1080/17461390701643026.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  116. Rojas FJ, Cepero M, Oña A, Gutierrez M. Kinematic adjustments in the basketball jump shot against an opponent. Ergonomics. 2000;43(10):1651–60. https://doi.org/10.1080/001401300750004069.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  117. Travassos B, Araújo D, Duarte R, McGarry T. Spatiotemporal coordination behaviors in futsal (indoor football) are guided by informational game constraints. Hum Mov Sci. 2012;31(4):932–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2011.10.004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  118. Dargo L, Robinson KJ, Games KE. Prevention of knee and anterior cruciate ligament injuries through the use of neuromuscular and proprioceptive training: an evidence-based review. J Athl Train. 2017;52(12):1171–2. https://doi.org/10.4085/1062-6050-52.12.21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  119. Stevenson JH, Beattie CS, Schwartz JB, Busconi BD. Assessing the effectiveness of neuromuscular training programs in reducing the incidence of anterior cruciate ligament injuries in female athletes: a systematic review. Am J Sports Med. 2015;43(2):482–90. https://doi.org/10.1177/0363546514523388.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  120. Heiderscheit BC, Hamill J, Emmerik REA. Variability of stride characteristics and joint coordination among individuals with unilateral patellofemoral pain. J Appl Biomech. 2002;18(2):110. https://doi.org/10.1123/jab.18.2.110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  121. Saltzman E, Kelso JA. Skilled actions: a task-dynamic approach. Psychol Rev. 1987;94(1):84–106. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.94.1.84.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  122. Scholz JP, Schöner G, Latash ML. Identifying the control structure of multijoint coordination during pistol shooting. Exp Brain Res. 2000;135(3):382–404. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002210000540.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  123. Crick F, Koch C. A framework for consciousness. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6(2):119–26. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn0203-119.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  124. Rycroft-Malone J, Bucknall T. Theory, frameworks, and models: laying down the groundwork. In: Rycroft-Malone J, Bucknall T, editors. Models and frameworks for implementing evidence-based practice: linking evidence to action. New York: Wiley; 2011. p. 23–50.

    Google Scholar 

  125. Bates BT. Single-subject methodology: an alternative approach. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1996;28(5):631–8. https://doi.org/10.1097/00005768-199605000-00016.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  126. Brown SJ, Nevill AM, Monk SA, Otto SR, Selbie WS, Wallace ES. Determination of the swing technique characteristics and performance outcome relationship in golf driving for low handicap female golfers. J Sports Sci. 2011;29(14):1483–91. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2011.605161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  127. James CR, Atkins LT, Dufek JS, Bates BT. An exploration of load accommodation strategies during walking with extremity-carried weights. Hum Mov Sci. 2014;35:17–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2014.03.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  128. James CR, Bates BT. Experimental and statistical design issues in human movement research. Meas Phys Educ Exerc Sci. 1997;1(1):55–69. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327841mpee0101_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  129. James CR, Bates BT, Dufek JS. Classification and comparison of biomechanical response strategies for accommodating landing impact. J Appl Biomech. 2003;19(2):106. https://doi.org/10.1123/jab.19.2.106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  130. Corcos DM, Gutman SR, Agarwal GC, Gottlieb GL. Movement strategies and the necessity for task differentiation. Behav Brain Sci. 1991;14(2):359–64. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00067157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  131. Cleather DJ, Cushion EJ. Muscular coordination during vertical jumping. J Hum Perform Health. 2019;1(1):1–10. https://doi.org/10.29359/JOHPAH.1.4.01.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  132. Cushion EJ, Warmenhoven J, North JS, Cleather DJ. Task demand changes motor control strategies in vertical jumping. J Mot Behav. 2021;53(4):471–82. https://doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2020.1797621.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  133. Rauch J, Leidersdorf E, Reeves T, Borkan L, Elliott M, Ugrinowitsch C. Different movement strategies in the countermovement jump amongst a large cohort of NBA players. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(17):6394. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17176394.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  134. Rein R, Button C, Davids K, Summers J. Cluster analysis of movement patterns in multiarticular actions: a tutorial. Mot Control. 2010;14(2):211–39. https://doi.org/10.1123/mcj.14.2.211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  135. Richter C, Franklyn-Miller A, King E, Falvey E. Biomechanical movement strategies in maximum effort change-of-direction. J Sci Med Sport. 2017;20:e63–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2017.01.168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  136. Sigurðsson HB, Briem K. Cluster analysis successfully identifies clinically meaningful knee valgus moment patterns: frequency of early peaks reflects sex-specific ACL injury incidence. J Exp Orthop. 2019;6(1):37. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40634-019-0205-5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  137. Donà G, Preatoni E, Cobelli C, Rodano R, Harrison AJ. Application of functional principal component analysis in race walking: an emerging methodology. Sports Biomech. 2009;8(4):284–301. https://doi.org/10.1080/14763140903414425.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  138. Wheat J, Baltzopoulos V, Milner Ce, Bartlett R, Tsaopoulos D, editors. Coordination variability during overground, treadmill and treadmill-on-demand running. In: 23 international symposium on biomechanics in sports; 2005.

  139. Wheat J, Milner CE, Bartlett RM. Kinematic variability during overground and treadmill running. J Sports Sci. 2004;22(3):245–6.

    Google Scholar 

  140. Magill RA. The classification of motor skills. In: Magill RA, editor. Motor learning and control: concepts and applications. 9th ed. New York: McGraw Hill; 2011. p. 2–21.

    Google Scholar 

  141. Romanchuk NJ, Del Bel MJ, Benoit DL. Sex-specific landing biomechanics and energy absorption during unanticipated single-leg drop-jumps in adolescents: implications for knee injury mechanics. J Biomech. 2020;113:110064. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2020.110064.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  142. Cossin M, Ross A, Prince F. Effect of jump heights, landing techniques, and participants on vertical ground reaction force and loading rate during landing on three different Korean teeterboards. Proc Inst Mech Eng. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1177/17543371211058031.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  143. Bressel E, Cronin J. The landing phase of a jump strategies to minimize injuries. J Phys Educ Recreat Dance. 2005;76(2):30–5. https://doi.org/10.1080/07303084.2005.10607332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  144. Ortega DR, Rodríguez Bíes EC, Berral de la Rosa FJ. Analysis of the vertical ground reaction forces and temporal factors in the landing phase of a countermovement jump. J Sports Sci Med. 2010;9(2):282–7.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  145. Boyer ER, Rooney BD, Derrick TR. Rearfoot and midfoot or forefoot impacts in habitually shod runners. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2014;46(7):1384–91. https://doi.org/10.1249/mss.0000000000000234.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  146. Hamill J, Gruber AH. Is changing footstrike pattern beneficial to runners? J Sport Health Sci. 2017;6(2):146–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jshs.2017.02.004.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  147. Meyer F, Falbriard M, Aminian K, Millet GP. How accurate is visual determination of foot strike pattern and pronation assessment. Gait Posture. 2018;60:200–2. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.12.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  148. Nunns M, House C, Fallowfield J, Allsopp A, Dixon S. Biomechanical characteristics of barefoot footstrike modalities. J Biomech. 2013;46(15):2603–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbiomech.2013.08.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  149. Shih Y, Lin KL, Shiang TY. Is the foot striking pattern more important than barefoot or shod conditions in running? Gait Posture. 2013;38(3):490–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2013.01.030.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  150. Kelso JA, Putnam CA, Goodman D. On the space-time structure of human interlimb co-ordination. Q J Exp Psychol A. 1983;35(Pt 2):347–75. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640748308402139.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  151. Carson HJ, Collins D, Richards J. Intra-individual movement variability during skill transitions: a useful marker? Eur J Sport Sci. 2014;14(4):327–36. https://doi.org/10.1080/17461391.2013.814714.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  152. Lee MCY, Chow JY, Komar J, Tan CWK, Button C. Nonlinear pedagogy: an effective approach to cater for individual differences in learning a sports skill. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(8): e104744. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0104744.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  153. Podraza JT, White SC. Effect of knee flexion angle on ground reaction forces, knee moments and muscle co-contraction during an impact-like deceleration landing: implications for the non-contact mechanism of ACL injury. Knee. 2010;17(4):291–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.knee.2010.02.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  154. Seay JF, Van Emmerik RE, Hamill J. Low back pain status affects pelvis-trunk coordination and variability during walking and running. Clin Biomech. 2011;26(6):572–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinbiomech.2010.11.012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  155. Albano D, Lambiase G, Romano B, Vastola R. Nonlinear analysis of knee kinematic variability after ACL reconstruction for the return to sport. J Phys Educ Sport. 2021;21(2):922–6. https://doi.org/10.7752/jpes.2021.02114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  156. Decker LM, Moraiti C, Stergiou N, Georgoulis AD. New insights into anterior cruciate ligament deficiency and reconstruction through the assessment of knee kinematic variability in terms of nonlinear dynamics. Knee Surg Sports Traumatol Arthrosc. 2011;19(10):1620–33. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00167-011-1484-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  157. Stergiou N, Moraiti C, Giakas G, Ristanis S, Georgoulis AD. The effect of the walking speed on the stability of the anterior cruciate ligament deficient knee. Clin Biomech. 2004;19(9):957–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinbiomech.2004.06.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  158. Kapreli E, Athanasopoulos S. The anterior cruciate ligament deficiency as a model of brain plasticity. Med Hypotheses. 2006;67(3):645–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2006.01.063.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  159. Fithian DC, Paxton LW, Goltz DH. Fate of the anterior cruciate ligament-injured knee. Orthop Clin North Am. 2002;33(4):621–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0030-5898(02)00015-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  160. Noyes FR, Matthews DS, Mooar PA, Grood ES. The symptomatic anterior cruciate-deficient knee. Part II: the results of rehabilitation, activity modification, and counseling on functional disability. J Bone Joint Surg Am. 1983;65(2):163–74. https://doi.org/10.2106/00004623-198365020-00004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  161. Noyes FR, Mooar PA, Matthews DS, Butler DL. The symptomatic anterior cruciate-deficient knee. Part I: the long-term functional disability in athletically active individuals. J Bone Joint Surg Am. 1983;65(2):154–62. https://doi.org/10.2106/00004623-198365020-00003.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  162. Dinstein I, Heeger DJ, Behrmann M. Neural variability: friend or foe? Trends Cogn Sci. 2015;19(6):322–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.04.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  163. Waschke L, Kloosterman NA, Obleser J, Garrett DD. Behavior needs neural variability. Neuron. 2021;109(5):751–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2021.01.023.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to acknowledge Dr. Kiran Ahuja for her assistance with the scoping review process. The authors also wish to acknowledge Ross Brosnan and Taylah Dickson for their practical insights into the proposed theoretical framework.

Funding

Funding for this research was provided in part by an Australian Government Research Training Program Scholarship. This was co-funded by the Tasmanian Institute of Sport and The University of Tasmania and provided as a living stipend for the first author.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JC performed the literature review. JC and SN performed final data extraction. JC wrote the first draft of the manuscript. MS, SN, JF, and PC all provided iterative revisions to the original manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jake Cowin.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

All images have consent for publication, and relevant copyright agreement forms are attached in application.

Competing interests

Jake Cowin, Sophia Nimphius, James Fell, Peter Culhane, and Matthew Schmidt declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Cowin, J., Nimphius, S., Fell, J. et al. A Proposed Framework to Describe Movement Variability within Sporting Tasks: A Scoping Review. Sports Med - Open 8, 85 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40798-022-00473-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40798-022-00473-4

Keywords